top of page

Lessons from our Wild Primate Kin

Writer: Karin SaksKarin Saks

Gismo and rehabilitation back into the Wild

Article published in Mail and Guardian

By Karin Saks

21.jpg

Scientist George Schaller wrote of gorillas: "The eyes have a language of their

own - being subtle and of emotion that in no other visible way affects the

expressions of the animal. I could see hesitation and uneasiness, curiosity and

boldness and annoyance." He believed it was impossible to observe other species,

and especially other primates, without interpreting their behaviour in human

terms. After all, we humans are primates too.

I’d recently fostered Gismo - an orphaned baby

baboon - for almost eight months. I was delighted when Damian van Gas - a baboon rehabilitator at a reserve called Mosdene - gave me the opportunity to rehabilitate Gismo back into the

wild.

He would join a troop of baboons - comprised of 17 individuals - which had been

successfully released more than three years earlier. Rehabilitation, if successful, would

be the perfect answer to Gismo's uncertain future - previously there had been

suggestions that he be put into an enclosure at a rehab centre.

While it is necessary for a rehabilitator to interact with the baboons to some

extent, keeping them away from humans and their associations - dwellings,

vehicles, crops - is an important factor of rehabilitation. The Mosdene troop

had been released far from human habitation, they were protected by the privacy

of the reserve and had not proved to be "problematic". Most of them had

experienced the best and worst aspects of human behaviour, and were well aware

of the dangers they could pose.

The analysis of DNA molecules, which carry hereditary traits, has shown that

humans, chimpanzees and bonobos share more than 99% of the same make-up. But

until the 1960s science followed the lead of Swedish naturalist Carl Linnaeus,

who was initially responsible for placing humans and other primates in separate

categories. He later came to regret this decision and admitted having created a

separate slot for humans for the sake of the church, in spite of the fact that

he did not know of any generic characteristics separating humans and apes.

I found that human language, print and other media, as well as the dysfunctional

primate representatives held in captivity in zoos and research laboratories

proved inadequate when it came to painting a picture of baboon life. First-hand

interaction with the Mosdene troop and the inevitable consequences of the

rehabilitation process - developing relationships with individuals and observing

troop dynamics - gave me a totally different insight.

Shirley C Strum, an American anthropologist who spent more than a decade

observing an olive baboon troop, found she had to discard many of her academic

notions because they conflicted with the lessons the baboons themselves taught.

After spending time with the baboons, I realised that the fear of being

anthropomorphic - projecting human attributes on to animals who don't share

those attributes - severely stunts our perception of primate life. Ironically,

using human words as opposed to baboon language when describing baboon life is,

in itself, peculiarly anthropomorphic. Our closeness to other primates confuses

the issue further.

Because the study of the emotions and psychology of animals can be filled with

projections, these important areas were initially ignored by scientists (for the

sake of convenience) in favour of more accessible factual information. As a

result, science came to deny animals many of their most relevant attributes.

Ethologist Konrad Lorenz complained that "one of the most customary and

hackneyed objections to which ethologists have to listen is that humans are

unique". We humans are limited in truly grasping the psychological and emotional

lives of other species, due to our conceit and self- imposed status of

privilege. If it is anthropomorphic to attribute human characteristics to

animals, it is chauvinistic not to attribute human traits to animals who have

them.

Baboon language is expressed through a series of facial expressions, sounds and

gestures punctuated with emotion - a system ranging in intensity that is

uncannily similar to our own non-verbal communication. Because of this shared

understanding, it soon became effortless to interact with individuals in the

troop. I learnt to adopt social strategies in much the same way that the baboons

did to achieve their goals within the complex hierarchal structure of the group.

My motivation was a stressless release for Gismo.

Currently classified as "problem animals", baboons are not protected. Once

branded as "vermin", they have been indiscriminately shot, poisoned, trapped for

use in laboratories and culled. If, as many people seem to believe, conservation

must first serve the betterment of humans, we have to deny our dependent,

symbiotic relationship with animals - a separatist attitude that will surely

destroy ourselves along with all else. How can it not?

The rehabilitation of baboons can teach us much about the rehabilitation of more

endangered primates. Yet how stable is the population of baboons? The Convention

on International Trade in Endangered Species (Cites) lists baboons under

Appendix II, which means they are regarded as a species that could become

endangered if trade is not controlled. But in spite of the strict requirements

imposed by Cites, no monitoring of baboon populations and their habitat is done

in South Africa.

As a result of ignorance, baboons are progressively facing eradication. The

general apathy about this state of affairs was illustrated when Minister of

Environmental Affairs and Tourism Pallo Jordan gave the go-ahead last year for

the exportation of more than 80 baboons to French pharmaceutical company Sanofi.

Rehabilitation is a necessary procedure to counteract the destructive impact of

human interference in the natural world. Having had little interaction with his

own kind, Gismo at first resisted becoming a full member of the Mosdene troop,

although many of the individuals persisted in changing this. As the days passed,

he gained confidence and formed stronger, sustainable bonds, and eventually -

after nine days of slow interaction - assumed the full mantle of baboon life.

I parted with him confident that his social and nutritional needs were finally

being met. He was consistently engaged in mutual grooming, bonding and play

activities. He'd been adopted into a sub-group and now had a mother, Dotty, and

three of her consort partners - the paternalistic Grobler, Rat and Alfred E -

for fathers. Added to these "family" figures were a number of juvenile

playmates, as well as a few adult allies. He'd quickly picked up the

communicative skills required for smooth interaction.

Today, Gismo is a fully accepted member, surviving alongside his own kind, free

in the wild.

This article has been published and may not be redistributed or republished in any form whatsoever

without prior written consent

Below: Fatso (from the Naboomspruit troop) greets Gismo and myself on the first day we met.

fatso1.jpg

I'm a paragraph. Click here to add your own text and edit me. It's easy.

© 2023 by RISING DRAGON. Proudly created with Wix.com
 

bottom of page